Monday, September 16, 2019
On Being Sane in Insane Places Essay
Have you ever been in a confrontation with someone where they ended with calling you a lunatic, crazy, or insane? It does not feel too great. But at least you know you are sane, and to everyone else around you, you seem ââ¬Å"normal.â⬠Can you walk down the street and visibly see a difference between a sane and insane person? There are some people however that are permanently labelled as ââ¬Å"abnormalâ⬠or ââ¬Å"insane.â⬠These people are diagnosed as ââ¬Å"mentally illâ⬠and are forever stigmatized and in some cases, dehumanized because of such labeling. Is it easy to distinguish the sane from the insane, or is it in fact the environment and contexts in which observers find these people? David L. Rosenhan formulated an experiment to investigate if psychiatrists could identify the difference between people who are mentally ill and those who are not. According to Rosenhan, ââ¬Å"there are a great deal of conflicting data on the reliability, utility, and meani ng of such terms as ââ¬Å"sanity,â⬠ââ¬Å"insanityâ⬠â⬠¦ normality and abnormality are not universalâ⬠(Rosenhan, 1973, pg.310). Rosenhan formed a group of 8 people who attempt to gain admission to 12 different hospitals across the United States. The psuedopatients each claimed of having textbook symptoms of schizophrenia. Once admitted, the psuedopatients each described their histories truthfully, and ââ¬Å"they strongly biased the subsequent results favor of detecting sanityâ⬠(pg.313). Each psuedopatient ââ¬Å"ceased simulating any symptoms of abnormalityâ⬠(pg.313). Nursing reports obtained that each pseudopatient ââ¬Å"exhibited no abnormal indicationsâ⬠(pg.313). The average length of hospitalization was 19 days, and upon discharge, each psuedopatient had a case summary written. All of the summaries were ââ¬Å"unintentionally distorted by the staff to achieve consistency with a popular theory of the dynamics of schizophrenic reactionâ⬠(pg.316). Each psuedopatient upon discharge was reported as in remission, not cured, which perpetuates the negative label. These ascribed labels are permanent, and puts the diagnosed person at a disadvantage forever. People diagnosed as mentally ill are treated dif ferently, in most cases they are treated as ifà their thoughts and opinions cannot be trusted or taken seriously. This is not fair, nor is it accurate. There is an ââ¬Å"enormous overlap in the behaviors of the sane and the insaneâ⬠(pg.318). The sane are not always sane, similarly, ââ¬Å"the insane are not always insaneâ⬠(pg.318). While reading this article, I was reminded about the homeless, and that many people assume that just because they are homeless, they are likely to have a mental illness. Now I see why many people assume this; the reason is the environment and contexts that civilians see these homeless people in. It is possible that some of these people are mentally ill, but it is more likely that they are in their position because of other reasons. From a sociological perspective, this experiment highlights the conflict perspective. In class, we have discussed how power is the core of all social relationships, all social systems feature a superordinate group and a subordinate group. In mental institutions, as well as in the public, conflict perspective is conveyed via sane people discriminating the allegedly insane people. Psychiatrists, nurses, and even civilians are above the mentally ill, because they are ââ¬Å"saneâ⬠while the ââ¬Å"insaneâ⬠are incapable of having valid opinions. Pow er is scarce, and everyone wants some of it, and unfortunately this permanent label is debilitating for the person who is marked with it, leaving them with little hope of upward mobility in our society. References Rosenhan, David L, (1973). On Being Sane in Insane Places. 310-321
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.